Academic performance and intelligence quotient of primary school children in Enugu
Uzoamaka Chinenye Akubuilo, Kenechukwu Kosisochukwu Iloh, Justus Uchenna Onu, Adaeze Chikaodinaka Ayuk, Agozie Chukwunedum Ubesie, Anthony Nnaemeka Ikefuna
Corresponding author: Kenechukwu Kosisochukwu Iloh, Department of Paediatrics, University of Nigeria Teaching Hospital, Ituku-Ozalla, Enugu, Nigeria
Received: 16 Apr 2020 - Accepted: 02 Jun 2020 - Published: 25 Jun 2020
Domain: Pediatrics (general)
Keywords: Academic performance, intelligence quotient, primary, school
©Uzoamaka Chinenye Akubuilo et al. Pan African Medical Journal (ISSN: 1937-8688). This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution International 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Cite this article: Uzoamaka Chinenye Akubuilo et al. Academic performance and intelligence quotient of primary school children in Enugu. Pan African Medical Journal. 2020;36:129. [doi: 10.11604/pamj.2020.36.129.22901]
Available online at: https://www.panafrican-med-journal.com//content/article/36/129/full
Academic performance and intelligence quotient of primary school children in Enugu
Uzoamaka Chinenye Akubuilo1, Kenechukwu Kosisochukwu Iloh1,2,&, Justus Uchenna Onu3, Adaeze Chikaodinaka Ayuk1,2, Agozie Chukwunedum Ubesie1,2, Anthony Nnaemeka Ikefuna1,2
&Corresponding author
Introduction: intellectual capacity measured as intelligence quotient (IQ) is one of the determinants of school performance of children. It influences academic achievement, future personal health, social well-being and therefore, is of public health significance. The objectives of the study was to determine the intelligence quotient (IQ) and academic performance of primary school children in Enugu-East LGA.
Methods: children who met the inclusion criteria were recruited from both public and private primary schools in the Local Government Area (LGA) using a proportionate multistage sampling technique. Academic performance was classified into high, average and low academic using past records of class assessment. Intelligence Quotient was assessed using the Raven´s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM) and was grouped into optimal and suboptimal. A semi-structured questionnaire was used to obtain data such as-age, gender, socio-economic indices and family size of the study participants. Analysis was done with Statistical Package for Social Sciences (IBM-SPSS).
Results: a total of 1,122 pupils aged 6 to 12 years were recruited. Optimal IQ and high academic performance was found in 54.0% and 58.8% of the study participants. Being from upper social class, in private school, and family size less than 4 were the significant determinants of high IQ and good academic performance (p<0.001).
Conclusion: low socio-economic status, large family size and public school attendance impact negatively on IQ and academic performance. Hence, measures to curb large family sizes (i.e.>4 children) and improve the socio-economic status of families are needed environmental measures to improve intelligence and academic performance.
Intelligence is the aggregate or global capacity of an individual to act purposefully, to think rationally and to deal effectively with the environment [1]. General intellectual functioning (referred to as intelligence quotient) typically refers to one´s global or overall level of intelligence [2]. Intelligence quotient (IQ) is critical for independent participation in core activities such as education, self-care, and in later life, employment and living independently [3]. Measures of ‘Global IQ’ reflect an individual's overall ‘ability to understand complex ideas, to adapt effectively to the environment, to learn from experience, to engage in various forms of reasoning, [and] to overcome obstacles by taking thought’ [4]. Intelligence is affected by both environmental and biologic factors [5]. Intelligence and academic performance are different but interrelated concepts as intelligence is said to be one of the most important cognitive factors responsible for the variations in achievement scores [6].
Intellectual capacity (measured as intelligence quotient or IQ) is said to be one of the determinants of poor school performance of children [5]. Academic achievement influences future personal health and is, therefore, of significant public health concerns [6]. Good education has been linked to better jobs, higher income, and higher socio-economic status; while poor school performance with its attendant risk of school dropout results in future income reductions and thereby perpetuates the inter-generational cycle of poverty. Still, there are conflicting reports in the literature regarding the relationship between IQ and academic performance [7-10]. Several factors such as nutrition, education, socio-economic status, age, gender, school type and family size may affect IQ and academic performance [11, 12]. This study aimed to determine the intelligence quotient and academic performance of primary school children in Enugu-East LGA, the correlation between the two as well as their relationship with socio-demographic variables (age, gender, socio-economic status, family-size and school-type).
This was a cross-sectional descriptive study involving school-aged children (6 to 12 years) in Enugu-East local government area of Enugu State. The study was carried out over a 3-month period from November 2017 to February 2018. Children who had attended same school for the three preceding terms, whose parents gave consent and the child assented to participate in the study if seven years and above were recruited into the study. Children who met the inclusion criteria were recruited from both public and private primary schools using a proportionate multistage sampling technique. In the first stage, simple random sampling by balloting was used to select one public and one private school from each political ward. In stage two, based on the total number of estimated pupils in each school, samples were allocated accordingly using the proportionate allocation method. With proportionate allocation method, the sample size of each school was proportionate to the population of the school. In stage three, the allocated sample size for each school was re-allocated proportionately across pupils in the different classes. In stage four, within each class, the pupils to be studied were selected randomly using a computer generated table of random numbers.
In computing the required sample size for the study, we used the findings from Adedeji et al. [13] for the following reasons: first, they studied similar age group using the similar study instruments. Second, both studies were carried out in the same country. Based on their finding of the prevalence of sub-optimal IQ of 62.5%, we computed the required sample size by using this Figure 1 to substitute in the Araoye [14] formula for a population <10000 and arrived at a minimum sample size of 1098.
Intelligence quotient was assessed using the Raven´s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM). The Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test (RSPM) is widely used as a general intelligence test in the world [15]. The Raven´s standardized progressive matrices (RSPM) was designed to measure a person´s ability to form perceptual relations and to reason by analogy independent of language and formal schooling, and may be used in persons ranging from six years to adulthood [16]. It has become one of the leading and most frequently used test of non-verbal or abstract reasoning and has been described as the paradigm test of non-verbal, abstract reasoning ability. This version has been effectively used and validated in Nigeria [9] and other African countries [17-20]. For ease of interpretation, RPM grades had been divided in two groups; optimal and suboptimal IQ [21].
The overall scores in percentage for each child in the three terminal examinations in four subjects (English language, Mathematics, Social studies and Primary science) during the completed academic year preceding the study period was documented and the average taken. This was used as an index of general academic performance. The performance was scored as high if ≥75 whereas 50-74% and <50% were scored as average and low respectively [22]. Those with a low overall score were regarded as having poor academic performance [22]. A semi-structured questionnaire was used to obtain data such as-age, gender, socio-economic indices and family size of the study participants. Data analyses were done using IBM-Statistical Package for Social Sciences (IBM-SPSS) version 20.0 (Chicago II). The normality of distribution of age, IQ raw scores and academic grades was checked using Shapiro-Wilk test. They were found to be normally distributed. The associations between intelligence quotient (categorized as optimal and sub-optimal)/academic performance (categorized as low, average, and high) and socio-demographic variables were tested using chi square. Binary and multinomial logistic regression was used to determine the strength of association between the socio-demographic variables that were significant at the univariate analysis and the intelligence quotients (optimal versus sub-optimal) and academic performance (low, average, and high), respectively. The relationship between IQ (optimal versus sub-optimal), age and academic performance (as continuous variables) was analyzed using independent t-test and effect size at 95% confidence interval was calculated. A p-value of less than 0.05 was regarded as significant.
Ethics approval and consent to participate: ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics and Research Committee of the University of Nigeria Teaching Hospital with protocol number NHREC/05/01/2008B-FWA00002458-IRB00002323 issued on the 7th of July, 2017. Informed consent was obtained from the parents of the children. The retrieved information was transferred into a private computer and passworded. Data was anonymized, and questionnaires had no names. Participation in the study was entirely voluntary, and no financial inducement whatsoever was involved. Participants were informed that voluntary withdrawal at any stage of interaction was guaranteed for them without any adverse effect. The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
General characteristics of the study population: a total of 1,122 pupils aged 6 to12 years were recruited from 10 public and 10 private schools in the local government. The mean age of the study participants was 9.0±1.7 years. The mean ages of the males and females were 9.0±1.7 and 9.0±1.6 years, respectively. The majority (53.6%) were females, from upper socio-economic class (39.1%), and in private school (53.9%). The prevalence of sub-optimal intelligence quotient among the study participants was 46.0%, whereas 58.8%, 35.3% and 5.9%, respectively had high, average and low academic performance (Table 1).
Relationship between socio-demographic variables and intelligence quotient: there was a significant relationship between social class and intelligence quotient (Χ2=84.4, p<0.001), such that participants from upper socio-economic class were 2.9 times more likely than those from lower social class to have higher IQ [AOR, (95%CI); 2.9(1.8-3.6]. In addition, participants from private schools and those whose family size was less than 4 were significantly more likely to have higher IQ [AOR, (95% CI); 3.7(2.9-4.8) and 1.4(1.0-2.1)], respectively (Table 2). Younger participants had significantly optimal IQ compared to older participants although with weak effect size (t=10.8, df=1120, p<0.001, effect size= -0.1).
Relationship between socio-demographic variables and academic performance: the association between socio-demographic characteristics and academic performance are shown in Table 3, Table 4, Table 5. There was a significant association between academic performance and gender (p=0.02) such that females were more likely than males to have higher academic performance AOR, (95% CI), 2.4(1.4-4.3)]. Being from upper social class (p<0.001), in private school (p=0.001), and family size less than 4 (p<0.001) were significantly associated with high academic performance (Table 6).
The proportion of children with optimal IQ in this study were more than those with suboptimal IQ. This differed from the observations of similar studies which assessed intelligence using the same tool. Ejekwu et al. [12]. in Enugu, Adedeji et al. [13] in Jos and Ijarotimi et al. [23] in Akure noted suboptimal intelligence in 69.7%, 62.7% and 83.4% of their study participants respectively. These variations with the current study could be explained by difference in methodology. Ejekwu et al. [12]. studied only public schools. The exclusion of children in the private schools limits generalization of their result. Although Adedeji et al. [13] and Ijarotimi et al. [23] like the index study, studied both private and public schools in the urban cities of Jos and Akure respectively, a greater proportion of their study participants were stunted and wasted. Ninety-four percent of the children in this study had an academic performance of at least 50%. The fact that most of these children performed above average academically could be related to the high rate of optimal intellectual ability. However, unlike the intellectual assessment which used a uniform tool, the academic assessment was not uniform as it differed from school to school. As a result, bias cannot be excluded. Using past school records as the index study, Adeyemi [24] in Osun noted a below average academic performance of 9.2% in private primary schools while Abebe et al. [25] in Ethiopia noted 23.2% of the pupils were below average. The result of this study compares with these studies as the majority had above average academic performance.
This study showed a significant association between age and intelligence quotient. Those who had optimal intelligence were significantly younger than those with suboptimal IQ. Ejekwu et al. [12] also noted that younger age group (less than 10 years) performed significantly better on Raven´s test. It´s been suggested that younger children pay better attention to instructions which may be responsible for their better performance.8 On the contrary, Survana et al. [26] noted a positive correlation of increasing intelligence score with age. They however assessed IQ using Standford Binet and very young people have been noted to perform poorly on this test as they lack the sustained concentration needed to complete the text [27]. Conversely, this study found that older children had a significantly better academic performance. This trend was also reported by Acham et al. [28] in Uganda. Abebe et al. [25] in Ethiopia also noted that children in grade 4 and above had higher odds of performing above average in academic achievement. This can be explained by the fact that sustained attention which increases with age is essential for effective learning and younger children lack this sustained attention. The association between IQ, academic performance and socio-economic status was significant in this study with the middle and lower class more likely to have suboptimal IQ than those in the upper class. Their odds of performing sub-optimally were about two to three times that of the upper class. Adedeji et al. [13] and Ejekwu et al. [12] observed that lower socio-economic indices significantly affected intelligence scores while Abebe et al. [25] and Ampofo et al. [29] documented significant relationship between academic performance and lower socio-economic status.
The relationship between socio-economic class, intelligence quotient and academic performance may be mediated by a third factor. The lower IQ in lower socio-economic class is largely attributable to the unfavourable environmental factors that usually accompany poverty. Indices of social disadvantage such as the risk of diseases/illnesses for example iron deficiency anaemia, under-nutrition and low level of parental education are common among persons in the lower socio-economic and could lead to impaired cognitive function [13, 30]. For example, Asmare et al. [30], found that indicators of under-nutrition are significantly associated with reduced intelligence and academic performance. Although nutritional status assessment was outside the scope of the present paper, previous reports have shown a positive correlation with intelligence and academic performance [13, 30]. Another possible mediator of the relationship between socio-economic class, intelligence quotient and academic performance may be parental education. In calculating the child´s social class, parental education and occupation is considered. In other words, the finding that children from upper socio-economic class were more likely to have higher intelligence quotient and academic performance may be related to a third factor-parental education. Bakar et al. [31] reported that children from families where parents are less educated tend to perform systematically worse in school than pupils whose parents have more education. In addition, these children are more likely both at home and in schools to be deprived of surroundings that stimulate proper cognitive development.
The result of this study showed that children in public schools were more likely to perform sub-optimally in the test of intelligence and to have a low academic performance. This trend was also documented by Adedeji et al. [13], Ijarotimi et al. [23] and Millones et al. [32]. This could be explained by the more conducive learning atmosphere observed in private schools which encourage intellectual stimulation and development. Academic performance was significantly better among females. This is similar to the findings by Duckworth A et al. [33] and Abebe et al. [25]. Females spend more time indoors and have better opportunities to study contrary to the males who spend a lot of time outdoors playing with their friends [25]. Boys are prone to disruptive and inattentive classroom behaviours that retard learning leading to a male educational disadvantage. Girls are also said to be more obedient than boys. They pay more attention to details than boys and as a result, are said to learn better than boys. In addition, the differences in gender with respect to academic performance may be mediated by emotional intelligence [34]. Generally, there literature is inconsistent with regards of gender differences with emotional intelligence (EI), but some authors have reported better EI with females. Emotional intelligence has been shown to be associated with more pro-social behaviours and better academic performance. Although not examined in this study, this gender differences in academic performance may have been influenced by the EI.
Age, low socio-economic status, large family size and public school attendance impact negatively on IQ and academic performance. Hence, measures to curb large family sizes and improve the socio-economic status of families should be put in place.
What is known about this topic
- There has been differing reports in the proportion of school-aged children with optimal intelligent quotient, and most of the studies studied intelligence quotient only and concentrated only in the government owned schools;
- There has been no consensus on the relationship between intelligence quotient and academic performance. Some authors noted a positive correlation where high IQ was associated with high academic performance, while some found no correlation between the two.
What this study adds
- This study has gone a step further to look at both intelligent quotient and academic performance in both government owned schools and private owned schools;
- Low socio-economic status, large family size and public school attendance impact negatively on IQ and academic performance;
- IQ and academic performance were positively correlated with each other.
The authors declare no competing interests.
UCA and KKI conceptualized the study and together with KKI, JUO, ACU and ANI developed the study methodology. Data collection, entry and analysis was by UCA, ACU and JUO. All the authors participated in the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
We are grateful to the children and their families that participated in this study.
Table 1: the socio-demographic variables of the study participants
Table 2: the relationship between socio-demographic variables and intelligence quotient
Table 3: the relationship between of socio-demographic variables and academic performance
Table 4: logistic regression of the socio-demographic predictors of intelligent quotients
Table 5: multinomial logistic regression of the association between academic performance and some socio-demographic parameters
Table 6: the relationship between age, intelligence quotient and academic performance
Figure 1: study flow chart (enrollment analysis)
- Wechsler D. The measurement of adult intelligence. 3rd ed. The Williams & Wilkins Company: Baltimore. 1944; 258. Google Scholar
- Hartman DE. Wechsler adult intelligence scale IV (WAIS IV): return of the gold standard. Appl Neuropsychol. 2009; 16(1):85-7. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Rebecca YF, Max G, Leanne MJ. Intelligence assessments for children with cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013 Oct;55(10):911-8. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Baron IS, and Leonberger KA. Assessment of intelligence in the preschool period. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012 Dec;22(4):334-44. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Mahnaz H, Gholamreza C, Maryam A, Parvin J, Setila D, Mehrab S. The Impact of undernutrition on intelligence quotient and impulsivity index. In J compr Ped. 2013;4(2):105-109. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Kaya F, Juntune J, Stough L. Intelligence and its relationship to achievement. Elementary Education online. 2015; 14:1060-1078. Google Scholar
- Davidson JE. The relationship between IQ scores and achievement is an imperfect one, with many exceptions to the rule. In Ormrod JE, Educational Psychology Developing Learners (eds). 2008; 155-156.
- Naderi H, Abdullah R, Hamid TA, Sharir J. Intelligence and academic performance: An investigation of gender differences. Life sci J.2010; 7(1):83-87. Google Scholar
- Naik SR, Itagi SK and Patil M. Relationship between nutrition status, intelligence and academic performance of Lambani school children of Bellary district, Karnataka. Intl J Farm sci. 2015; 5:259-267. Google Scholar
- Hamid JJ, Amal KM, Hasmiza H, Pim CD, Ng LO, Wan M. Effect of gender and Nutritional status on Academic achievement and cognitive function among primary school children in a rural District in Malaysia. Mal J Nutri. 2011; 17(2):189-200. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Jaeger M. Do large sibships really lead to lower educational attainment? New evidence from quasi-experimental variation in couples´ reproductive capacity. Acta social. 2008; 51(3):217-235. Google Scholar
- Ejekwu, AD, Ene-Obong, H, Oguizu O J. Nutritional status and cognitive performance among children aged 5-12 years from urban and rural areas of Enugu state. Afr J psycho study soc Issues. 2012; 15(2):481-496. Google Scholar
- Adedeji IA, John C, Okolo SN, Ebonyi AO, Abdu H, Bashir MF. Malnutrition and the Intelligence Quotient of Primary School Pupils in Jos, Nigeria. British Journal of Medicine and Medical Research. 2017; 21(2):1-13. Google Scholar
- Araoye MO. Research methodology with statistics for health and social sciences. Ilorin: Nathadex Publishers. 2003; 115-129. Google Scholar
- Ven AHGS, van der Ellis JL. A Rasch analysis of Raven´s Standard Progressive Matrices. Personality and Individual Differences. 2000; 29: 45-64. Google Scholar
- Raven JC, Court JH, Raven JC. Manual for Raven´s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales - Section 1: General Overview. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.1993. Google Scholar
- Iloh KK, Emodi IJ, Ibeziako NS, Ikefuna AN, Ubesie AC, Iloh ON et al. Neurocognitive Function of School-aged HIV-infected Children in Enugu, Nigeria. J Trop Pediatr. 2017; 63(6):425-430. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Klingelhofer EL. Performance of Tanzanian secondary school pupils on the Raven´s Progressive Matrices Test. J Soc Psychol. 1967; 72:205-215. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Smith L, Adnams C, Eley B. Neurological and Neurocognitive Function of HIV-Infected Children Commenced On Antiretroviral Therapy. S Afr J Child Helth. 2008; 2:108-113. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Daley T, Whaley S, Sigman MD, Espinosa MP, Neumann. IQ on the Rise: The Flynn Effect in Rural Kenyan Children. Psychol Sci. 2003; 14: 215-219. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Boyede GO, Lesi FEA, Ezeaka CV, Umeh CS. The Neurocognitve Assessment of HIV-Infected School-Aged Nigeria Children. W J AIDS. 2013;3:124-130. Google Scholar
- Ogunfowora OB, Olanrewaju DM, Akenzua GI. A comparative study of academic achievement of children with sickle cell anaemia. J Natl Med Assoc. 2005; 97(3):405-408. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Ijarotimi OS, Ijadunola KT. Nutritional status and intelligence quotient of primary school children in Akure community of Ondo state, Nigeria. Tanzan Health Res Bull. 2007; 9(2):69-76. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Adeyemi SB. Comparative study of pupils academic performance between private and public primary schools. World Journal of Education. 2014; 4(4):55-60. Google Scholar
- Abebe F, Geleto A, Sena L, Hailu C. Predictors of academic performance with due focus on undernutrition among students attending primary schools of Hawa Gelan district, Southwest Ethiopia: a school based cross sectional study. BMC Nutrition. 2017; 3:30. PubMed | Google Scholar
- Survana, Itagi SK. Nutritional status and level of intelligence of school children. Karnataka J Agric Sci. 2009; 22(4):874-876. Google Scholar
- Oladimeji Y. The progressive matrices. In: Oladimeji Y (eds), Psychological Assesment Techniques in health care. Ist ed Obafemi Awolowo university press limited. 2005.221-24.
- Acham H, Kikafunda J, Oluka S, Malde M, Tyleskar T. Height, weight, body mass index and learning achievement in Kumi district, East of Uganda. Sci Res Essay. 2008; 3:1-8. Google Scholar
- Ampofo E, Osei-Owusu B. Determinants of academic performance among senior high school students in the ashanti mampong municipality of Ghana. Eur J Res Reflect Educ Sci. 2015; 3(3):33-48. Google Scholar
- Asmare B, Taddele M, Berihun S, Wagner F. Nutritional status and correlation with academic performance among primary school children, Northwest, Ethopia. BMC Res Notes 2018; 11:805. Google Scholar
- Bakar NH, Mamat I, Ibrahim M. Influence of parental education on academic performance of secondary school students in Kuala Terengganu. Int J Acad. 2017; 7(8). Google Scholar
- Millones MD, Leeuwen KV. Academic performance of Peruvian elementary school children: The case of schools in Lima at the 6th grade. Interdisciplinaria. 2011; 28(2):323. Google Scholar
- Duckworth A, Seligman M. Self-discipline gives girls the edge: gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. J Edu Psychol. 2006;98 (1),198-208. Google Scholar
- Chew BH, Zain AM, Hassan F. Emotional intelligence and academic performance in first and final year medical students: a cross-sectional study. BMC Med Educ. 2013; 13:44. PubMed | Google Scholar